Abstract

A vicious cycle exists when higher global temperatures increase the demand for indoor air-conditioning, which consumes significant energy while heating the outdoors. These higher outdoor temperatures then prompt more air-conditioning use. This unsustainable cycle motivated us to develop an intervention to encourage more energy-efficient temperature adjustments during warm ambient conditions. We explored whether an experimental thermostat interface, which incorporated mechanical fans, affected individual thermostat-setting behavior. Experimental parameters were (1) feel versus don't-feel fans and (2) high- versus low-visibility fans. Participants were 23 university students, including 20 enrolled in an introductory psychology course. When prompted to make temperature adjustments, we found that participants who felt the fans selected higher (more energy-efficient) temperatures in warm ambient conditions. This effect held regardless of whether participants could clearly see the fans or not. These results inform how products can be designed to increase energy-conscious behaviors.

1 Introduction and Motivation

Higher global temperatures increase the demand for indoor air-conditioning (A/C) [1,2]. This increased A/C use not only consumes significant energy but also further warms the planet, leading to a vicious cycle. Notably, recent heat waves have increased power generation from coal-fired stations to help meet extra A/C demand [3]. Furthermore, A/C use significantly contributes to greenhouse gas emissions due to widely used refrigerants with high global warming potential [4]. The growing environmental harms of increased A/C demand motivated us to explore how to decrease its use.

Much A/C use is driven by current thermal comfort standards, i.e., temperatures are set lower than what is required to maintain health [5]. Thus, we devised and explored an intervention that could improve comfort at the time of temperature adjustment. Specifically, we examined the potential role of mechanical fans as part of the thermostat interface. Fans enhance cooling by increasing convective heat transfer. In general, fans deliver instant, targeted thermal relief, while consuming significantly less electricity than using A/C to cool an entire indoor space [6]. The current study proposes using mechanical fans, not to cool an entire space, but as an intervention while thermostat adjustments are being made.

While the role of information in increasing environmentally conscious behavior has been extensively studied, we wanted to explore product features that can increase such behavior. Other research supports that a product’s design can significantly affect environmentally conscious practices. For example, our laboratory’s past work suggests that products which present resources in discrete units rather than continuous flows can reduce resource consumption [7]. Srivastava and Shu also examined the role of product affordances on eco-conscious behavior [8]. Telenko et al. compiled 76 design-for-environment guidelines, which include, “Incorporate features that prevent or discourage waste of materials by the user” [9]. Withanage et al. incorporated behavioral observations to design for sustainable appliance use [10]. She and MacDonald examined how a product’s sustainability triggers affect pro-environmental decisions [11]. Pakravan and MacCarty integrated usage context and user behavior in designing for clean-technology adoption [12]. Adding to this body of work, the current research examined whether mechanical fans can be used to affect temperature-adjustment behavior during thermostat interaction.

Before describing the details of our study, below we summarize existing technologies and their limitations as context for our approach.

1.1 Existing Technologies and Limitations.

While the primary function of household thermostats is to regulate temperature according to a setpoint, energy efficiency and cost savings have become common, secondary goals [13]. Thus, thermostat technologies, e.g., smart, learning-, schedule-, occupancy-, and feedback-based strategies, have been developed to conserve energy. For example, smart thermostat technologies, such as the Nest, learn and generate a schedule based on user-set temperature adjustments [14]. However, schedule-learning functionality does not necessarily produce an energy-efficient schedule, particularly if user-specific temperature adjustments are inefficient [15]. Furthermore, Yang and Newman attributed energy savings to participants’ behavior, and not the thermostat’s automated functionalities [14]. Therefore, the energy-saving potential of schedule-learning smart thermostats is limited by users’ motivations to enact eco-conscious adjustments.

As another example, feedback-based interfaces reportedly provide users with increased usability and practical energy-saving recommendations. The ThermoCoach monitors occupancy patterns and recommends schedules to minimize energy consumption [16]. Three types of schedules are suggested to the user: (1) high comfort, (2) energy saver, and (3) super energy saver, alongside their current schedule. The user can then choose to activate one of these schedules. In a 12-week field study comparing users’ pre-intervention schedule to ThermoCoach’s recommended scheduling, the interface saved 4.7% more energy than a programable thermostat and 12.4% more energy than the Nest thermostat. Thus, a feedback-based interface can conserve energy, but still relies on the user to choose an energy-saving schedule.

Overall, the promise of energy-saving technologies has been limited by user behavior. That is, when experiencing thermal discomfort in the moment, users can override these systems and continue to set energy-inefficient temperatures.

1.2 Strategies to Encourage Pro-environmental Behavior.

Many frameworks exist to describe behavior-change strategies to increase sustainabilty [17,18]. One categorization method differentiates between informational and structural strategies [17]. Informational strategies aim to change individuals’ perceptions and motivations [19]. In contrast, structural strategies change the circumstances wherein decisions are made.

Pro-environmental behavior (PEB) interventions can also be considered on a range based on user versus product in control. Both extremes of this range, information-based at one end and automation/forcing at the other end, have limited effectiveness or may create new challenges [18]. Products that focus on informational strategies, e.g., by providing feedback, enable a high degree of user control [20,21]. For example, the ThermoCoach suggests energy-saving schedules [16,22], but a motivated user must activate a schedule to realize energy savings. At the other end of the range, highly automated smart thermostats can increase user convenience [23], but often at the cost of limited user control and lack of understanding around automated adjustments [14].

Interventions that aim to change beliefs, values, and motivations assume that people are rational agents who make reasoned decisions [17,24,25]. However, time, availability of information, and human computational power all significantly limit people’s ability to engage in effortful decision-making [24,26,27].

Another approach to encourage PEB involves changing people’s external circumstances [17,18]. Doing so may have downstream effects on individuals’ (1) perception of their abilities, (2) habits and behavioral patterns, (3) expectations of the self and others, and (4) attitudes, beliefs, and values [18].

Changing the physical environment to enable PEB is a valuable behavior-change strategy, as it does not rely on underlying beliefs and attitudes. For example, cigarette littering was reduced by 64% with structural interventions alone, e.g., by adding ashtrays and litter bins [28]. This significant change in littering behavior was achieved without altering attitudes on littering. As another example, a water-conservation intervention that reduced the rate of waste-water outflow was found to be effective, regardless of whether people reported practicing PEBs in general [29].

Such results support the design of interventions that do not rely on pro-environmental attitudes and intentions. More generally, external factors that can be changed to increase PEB include the: (1) physical environment, including infrastructure and facilities, (2) availability of products and product characteristics, and (3) incentives and constraints [17,18].

Withanage et al. classified wasteful behavior as energy overuse failure modes, which are attributed to high-energy-consuming habits. Such wasteful consumption can be reduced through either product design or behavior-change strategies [10].

For the current work, habitual energy-inefficient temperature setting to achieve comfort cooling can become the default choice [24], unless that choice is challenged or the decision environment is altered. An opportunity is identified by the above gap, which can be addressed through product design. That is, improved user interaction with products can increase energy-efficient behavior [9].

Specific to thermal comfort, energy-inefficient behavior can be driven by sensory stimuli from the environment, i.e., one feels hot, so adjusts the thermostat. Thus, altering the physical environment wherein temperature decisions are made is a promising behavior-change strategy.

We hypothesized that alleviating thermal discomfort during user-thermostat interaction may prompt behavior-change toward more energy-efficient temperature adjustments (i.e., higher indoor temperatures in warm ambient conditions). Therefore, we tested whether experiencing the cooling sensation of a fan, while setting the thermostat, would lead to more sustainable temperature selections, which could ultimately reduce energy consumption. Our results would inform the potential redesign of thermostats to reduce A/C use.

2 Materials and Methods

Many behavior-intervention studies elicit and report on participant intent [30]. For example, study participants are asked to complete statements to express intended behaviors, e.g., “Based on this, I will likely …” and “I intend to …” [31,32]. This approach is not ideal, as intention may not lead to actual behavior. Therefore, our study examined actual behavior to determine the efficacy of a fan-based experimental thermostat interface.

2.1 Study Environment.

Our fan-based intervention interfaces were tested in-person at a university laboratory, where the temperature ranged from 24 °C to 27 °C (75.2 °F to 80.6 °F) and relative humidity (RH) ranged from 30% to 63%. The laboratory’s indoor temperatures varied due to lack of effective A/C. However, participants arrived at the laboratory from an outdoor environment, also of significantly varying conditions. For example, on a representative day, outdoor temperature varied from 20.1 °C to 33.9 °C (68.2 °F to 93.0 °F) and RH ranged from 40% to 80%.

The laboratory consisted of two adjoining rooms. Participants would interact with an interface in one room, while the research assistant prepared the next interface, out of participants’ view, in the adjoining room. Four experimental interfaces were prepared according to participants’ randomly assigned order of conditions. After interacting with each interface, participants would complete a filler task (described below). The research assistant then directed the participant to the adjoining room to interact with the next interface.

2.2 Choice and Mounting of Physical Thermostat.

To approximate the real-world conditions of setting a thermostat, participants were asked to adjust temperature using a physical thermostat. This contrasts with specifying temperature adjustments in questionnaires [33] or through participant–researcher communication [34]. However, the physical characteristics of the study location made it infeasible to achieve the selected temperatures while the participant was undergoing the study. Thus, the thermostats did not affect the room temperature.

Most manufacturers recommend that residential thermostats be mounted 52 to 60 in. from the ground to account for natural air movement. To approximate this height, the experimental thermostats were mounted onto an existing structure that is 20 in. wide and 52 in. high. The top of this structure also enabled placement of the intervention-interface fans (∼5 in. deep) at face level.

LuxPro PSD011B pro-spec digital thermostats were selected due to their simplistic interface (Fig. 1 (left)). The large digital display only showed the current temperature, which could be adjusted with up-and-down arrow-shaped physical buttons. The thermostat had a temperature control range of 7–32 °C (44.6–89.6 °F). Two side switches and labels for fan setting (auto/on) and thermostat mode (heat/off/cool) were covered with white electrical tape so that participants only saw the temperature-adjustment buttons.

Fig. 1
LuxPro PSD011B pro-spec digital thermostat (left) and two 7-in.-diameter Xuenair (N2 Night Fan HND-N2) fans (right)
Fig. 1
LuxPro PSD011B pro-spec digital thermostat (left) and two 7-in.-diameter Xuenair (N2 Night Fan HND-N2) fans (right)
Close modal

2.3 Choice and Configuration of Fans.

Each experimental interface used a pair of 7-in.-diameter Xuenair (N2 Night Fan HND-N2) fans, shown in Fig. 1 (right). These fans had three speeds, and a maximum power of 5.5 W, using 4000 mAh batteries that were charged using a Type-C universal serial bus (USB) interface. For each study condition, a pair of fans (in below-described configurations) was placed directly above the thermostat, atop the structure on which the thermostat was mounted.

Table 1 summarizes the four fan configurations that comprised the study conditions. These conditions involved participants adjusting the experimental thermostat interface while fans were either (1) off or directed air (2a) unobstructed, (2b) behind a ventilation grill, or (2c) behind a plexiglass case that blocked all airflow. These configurations enabled exploration of how the state of the fan (feel and/or visibility) affects temperature adjustments. In all four conditions, a white-noise generator was used to mask different levels of fan noise.

Table 1

Study conditions

Feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV)Feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV)Don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV)Don't-feel-fan low-visibility (DFF + LV)
Fan feelFeel fanFeel fanDon't feel fanDon't feel fan
Fan visibility and locationHigh visibility, in front of clear coverLow visibility, behind ventilation grillHigh visibility, behind clear coverLow visibility, behind ventilation grill
Representation of interface
graphic
graphic
graphic
graphic
Actual experimental themostat interface
graphic
graphic
graphic
graphic
Feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV)Feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV)Don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV)Don't-feel-fan low-visibility (DFF + LV)
Fan feelFeel fanFeel fanDon't feel fanDon't feel fan
Fan visibility and locationHigh visibility, in front of clear coverLow visibility, behind ventilation grillHigh visibility, behind clear coverLow visibility, behind ventilation grill
Representation of interface
graphic
graphic
graphic
graphic
Actual experimental themostat interface
graphic
graphic
graphic
graphic

2.4 Study Conditions.

A 2 × 2 factorial design was used to examine the effect on temperature adjustments of two independent variables:

  1. fan feel, i.e., feel-fan (FF) versus don't-feel-fan (DFF) and

  2. fan visibility, i.e., high-visibility (HV) versus low-visibility (LV).

In the feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) condition, there were neither physical nor visual obstructions between the fans and participants.

In the feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV) condition, the fans were placed behind a white ventilation grill. While this condition was intended to hide the fans from participants’ view, they could partially see them behind the ventilation grill. Therefore, this condition was denoted low-visibility instead of no-visibility.

In the don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV) condition, the fans were placed behind a clear acrylic case so they could be seen as on, but participants could not feel the fans’ effects.

Finally, in the don't-feel-fan low-visibility (DFF + LV) condition, the fans were set at the off position. Again, because the fans were placed behind a ventilation grill, participants could partially see the fans.

2.5 Participants.

Twenty-five university-student participants were recruited for the study. Two participants were removed because they were unable to follow the study instructions. Of the remaining 23 participants, 20 were enrolled in the participant pool for an introductory psychology course, and 3 were part of the general university student population. Twenty-one were between the ages of 18 and 24, and two were between the ages of 25 and 34. Eleven identified as female, 11 identified as male, and one identified as gender queer.

2.6 Study Design and Condition Order.

Since temperature preference and thermal sensation effects are highly subjective and variable, a within-subject design was used to reduce individual differences between conditions. As described above, a 2 × 2 factorial design was used to examine the effect on temperature adjustments of two independent variables: (1) fan feel, i.e., feel-fan (FF) versus don't-feel-fan (DFF) and (2) fan visibility, i.e., high-visibility (HV) versus low-visibility (LV). Participants were randomly assigned to one of the two condition orders where fans provided either delayed versus immediate thermal relief.

  1. Delayed fan-effect: DFF + LV, FF + LV, DFF + HV, and FF + HV.

  2. Immediate fan-effect: FF + HV, FF + LV, DFF + LV, and DFF + HV.

The two orders were used to explore whether there was an effect of exposing participants to different sequences of conditions. The study took approximately 45 min to complete. The main factor in completion time was language competency. Due to in-person COVID-19 protocols, participants were required to wear face masks.

2.7 Study Procedure, Components, and Administration.

Participants were prompted to think about their thermal comfort before interacting with the thermostat interface. They first set an initial baseline temperature, which was compared with temperatures they set in four intervention conditions, ordered as described above. This procedure was devised to approximate a naturalistic temperature-adjustment environment.

Time was not specifically allocated for participants to acclimate to the study environment. Acclimation was not prioritized in order to mimic a target user who may interact with their thermostat in a state of mild-to-moderate thermal discomfort, i.e., after recently arriving from a warm outdoor environment. Additionally, the effect of each intervention interface on temperature adjustments could be more realistically measured during thermal discomfort. However, participants spent about 5 min receiving initial instructions before interacting with the intervention interface and setting a baseline temperature. Participants were then given a basic tutorial on how to operate the study thermostat. Overall, participants were not given time limits and could take as long as required to complete the tasks.

2.7.1 Initial Instructions and Baseline Temperature Setting.

Participants were asked to bring a laptop or tablet to the study location. Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants were informed that they would be testing different thermostat interfaces for a product-design study. Participants then used their devices to (1) read and sign the consent form, (2) complete a basic demographic questionnaire, and (3) complete an evaluation of their thermal comfort. Next, participants set their baseline temperature before interacting with the intervention interfaces located in the adjoining laboratory room.

During baseline temperature collection, participants were asked to complete four tasks:

  • Task 1: Think about what a comfortable temperature would be and set the thermostat to that temperature (the set temperature was recorded by a research assistant).

  • Task 2: Answer, “What feature stood out the most about the thermostat? Did that affect how you used it?”

  • Task 3: Answer, “What factors led you to select that temperature?”

  • Task 4: Answer, “Why did you set the thermostat to the setting that you did?”

Participants answered the questions for tasks 2–4 on qualtricsTM, a survey software. After the above four tasks for the baseline condition, participants completed a filler task where they counted syllables from randomly generated lists of words. A filler task was used to reduce carryover effects from one condition to the next. In the meantime, a research assistant prepared the subsequent interface configuration in the adjoining laboratory room. Next, participants proceeded to complete tasks on the four intervention interfaces in series.

2.7.2 Intervention-Interface Interaction.

After participants entered the adjoining laboratory room, they were provided instructions printed on paper. These instructions described study tasks 1 and 2 (detailed below), which they were asked to perform. The papers were collected by the research assistant upon task completion. Participants answered questions corresponding to tasks 3–5 on qualtricsTM.

The research assistant instructed participants to stand in a 40 cm × 30 cm (15.7 in. × 11.8 in.) rectangle taped on the floor, at a typical distance where users interact with thermostats. The research assistant then asked participants to complete the following five tasks:

  • Task 1: Take 30 s to think about your thermal comfort in this moment. Please write down how you are thermally feeling as you stand in the rectangle.

  • Task 2: Think about what a comfortable temperature would be and set the thermostat to that temperature (the set temperature was recorded by the research assistant).

  • Task 3: Answer, “What feature stood out the most about the thermostat? Did that affect how you used it?”

  • Task 4: Answer, “What factors led you to select that temperature?”

  • Task 5: Answer, “Why did you set the thermostat to the setting that you did?”

After each condition, participants completed a filler task of counting syllables in randomly generated lists of words. A different set of words was provided following each condition.

Pilot testing had revealed some sources of participant confusion. Many participants expected an instant change in room temperature or fan state after they set a temperature. Some participants removed the mounted thermostat and/or fans to play with them. To reduce this confusion and behavior, the following was posted next to the intervention interfaces:

Important information:

  • Do not remove thermostat

  • Changing the thermostat temperature does not guarantee change in room temperature

  • You cannot control the fan through the thermostat

2.7.3 Post-Intervention-Interface Assessments.

After undergoing all four conditions, participants completed assessments on:

  1. Their environment, to assess perception of indoor air quality, including perceived humidity, stuffiness, smell, and draft.

  2. Manipulation checks, effect perception, and feedback, to gauge participants’ understanding and experience with the intervention interfaces. Participants were also asked about the intervention interfaces’ efficacy in motivating them to set more energy-efficient temperatures.

  3. Value orientations, following Steg et al. [35], that included 16 items: three on hedonic values, five on egoistic values, four on altruistic values, and four on biospheric values.

  4. General study feedback, to elicit comments from participants in open-ended (free-text entry) format.

  5. Energy myths, to explore whether holding the incorrect conceptual model affected temperature selection. Corresponding questions were based on Pritoni et al.’s assessment of knowledge regarding energy myths [36].

  6. Post-thermal comfort using the American Society of Heating, Refrigerating and Air-Conditioning Engineers (ASHRAE) 7-point thermal sensation scale [37].

Assessments 2–4 were exploratory, and responses were intended to inform future research.

3 Results

Two-way analysis of variances (ANOVAs) were used to analyze the effects of fan feel, fan visibility, and the interaction between these two effects on both raw temperature selections and temperature shift. Temperature shift is defined as the difference between the temperature each participant set during a condition and the baseline temperature the participant set before any conditions. To assess differences in raw temperature selections between conditions, a repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted.

3.1 Effect of Fan Feel and Fan Visibility on Mean Temperature Shift.

A two-way ANOVA was performed to analyze the effect of fan feel, fan visibility, and their interaction on temperature shift. Comparing conditions where participants felt the fan or not, our results show that feeling the fan significantly altered temperature selection, F(1, 88) = 5.73, p = 0.02 (Fig. 2). That is, participants selected higher temperatures when they felt the fan (1.72 °C/3.10 °F) than when they did not (0.56 °C/1.01 °F). There was no significant main effect for fan visibility (Fig. 3), nor any significant interaction between fan feel and fan visibility.

Fig. 2
Mean temperature shifts (condition-baseline) by fan feel (*p < 0.05); error bars: standard error of the mean
Fig. 2
Mean temperature shifts (condition-baseline) by fan feel (*p < 0.05); error bars: standard error of the mean
Close modal
Fig. 3
Mean temperature shifts (condition-baseline) by fan visibility; error bars: standard error of the mean
Fig. 3
Mean temperature shifts (condition-baseline) by fan visibility; error bars: standard error of the mean
Close modal

3.2 Effect of Fan Feel and Fan Visibility on Mean Raw Temperature Selections.

A two-way ANOVA was performed to analyze the effect of fan feel, fan visibility, and the interaction between these two effects on raw temperature selections. Comparing conditions where study participants felt the fan or not, our results show that feeling the fan significantly altered temperature selection, F(1, 88) = 8.78, p = 0.004 (Fig. 4). Our results showed a marginally significant effect on temperature selection for fan visibility, F(1, 88) = 2.98, p = 0.09 (Fig. 5). There was no significant interaction between fan feel and fan visibility.

Fig. 4
Mean raw temperatures by fan feel (**p < 0.01); error bars: standard error of the mean
Fig. 4
Mean raw temperatures by fan feel (**p < 0.01); error bars: standard error of the mean
Close modal
Fig. 5
Mean raw temperatures by fan visibility (†p < 0.10); error bars: standard error of the mean
Fig. 5
Mean raw temperatures by fan visibility (†p < 0.10); error bars: standard error of the mean
Close modal

Specifically, participants’ mean raw temperature selections for the two conditions when they felt the fan (23.9 °C/75.0 °F) were higher than for the two conditions when they did not (22.5 °C/72.5 °F). With respect to visibility, participants’ mean raw temperature settings were marginally higher when fan visibility was high (23.5 °C/74.3 °F) than when it was low (22.8 °C/73.0 °F).

3.3 Mean Raw Temperatures Between Conditions.

A repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted to assess differences in raw temperature selections between conditions. Mauchly’s test indicated a violation of the sphericity assumption, χ2(9) = 26.05, p = 0.002. Since sphericity is violated (ɛ = 0.653), Greenhouse–Geisser corrected results are reported. Temperature selection was significantly affected by intervention interface, F(2.61, 57.43) = 9.18, p < 0.001 (Fig. 6). Post-hoc analysis with a Bonferroni adjustment revealed that temperature selections were significantly higher in the feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) condition than baseline (p = <0.001, 95% confidence interval (CI) = [−3.42, −1.35]). Temperature selections were also significantly higher in the feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) condition than all other conditions, i.e., feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV) (p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−2.05, −.75]), don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV) (p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−2.76, −1.08]), and don't-feel-fan low-visibility (DFF + LV) (p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−3.04, −.97]).

Fig. 6
Mean raw temperatures by condition (***p < 0.001); error bars: standard error of the mean
Fig. 6
Mean raw temperatures by condition (***p < 0.001); error bars: standard error of the mean
Close modal

The feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) condition had an average temperature setting of 24.4 °C/76.0 °F, which was significantly higher than the average baseline setting (22.0 °C/71.7 °F). The average temperature selections for the feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) condition were also significantly higher than the feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV) condition (23.2 °C/73.7 °F), don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV) condition (22.7 °C/72.8 °F), and don't-feel-fan low-visibility (DFF + LV) condition (22.4 °C/72.4).

Finally, there was no significant effect of order, i.e., the order of conditions that participants underwent did not affect their temperature-setting behavior.

4 Discussion

Results are discussed before turning to design implications.

4.1 Effect of Fan Feel and Visibility on Temperature Selection.

This study supports that feeling a fan can encourage energy-efficient temperature adjustments during warm-weather thermostat interaction. When comparing temperature shifts (between conditions and baseline), participants selected relatively higher temperatures in the two feel-fan conditions (1.72 °C/3.10 °F) than in the two don't-feel-fan conditions (0.56 °C/1.01 °F). In terms of mean raw temperature selections, participants set higher temperatures when they felt the fan (23.9 °C/75.0 °F) than when they did not feel the fan (22.5 °C/72.5 °F), a difference of 1.4 °C/2.5 °F.

Increased air speed is an effective intervention towards prompting energy-efficient temperature adjustments. Specifically, low-powered fans alone afforded thermal relief during user-thermostat interaction. The effect of air movement on expanding user thermal comfort is empirically supported. For example, Zhai et al. examined the effect of air movement using floor fans on (1) thermal comfort, (2) thermal sensation, and (3) perceived air quality (PAQ) while varying temperature and RH [38]. The authors concluded that air movement can maintain thermal comfort up to 30 °C at 60% RH, and PAQ can be maintained up to 30 °C at 80% RH. Thus, mechanical fans provide an energy-efficient means toward thermal comfort in warm and humid environments by expanding thermal tolerance.

The current-study participants’ qualitative responses also support the effect of increased air speed on whole-body thermal perception. Specifically, when asked to reflect on their thermal comfort, 20 of 46 responses in both feel-fan conditions indicated improved thermal comfort and/or satisfaction. The fan’s cooling effect is summarized by one participant’s response as, “Very much enjoying the breeze. Breeze hits larger area of my body, cooling it. Not as concerned about warm ambient temperature as before [when not interacting with interface].” Therefore, as expected, fans alleviated the thermal discomfort associated with warmer-than-neutral indoor conditions.

In contrast, a minority of participants reported thermal discomfort due to the fan-created draft. This undesired cooling also affected temperature-adjustment behavior. Seven of 46 responses in both feel-fan conditions included references to thermal discomfort. One participant response clarified how this may affect behavior as, “I feel cooler since there is a fan. It is a little bit too cold for me, so I might set the temperature higher. I had to pull down my sleeves because it is a bit too cool.” For participants already feeling thermally neutral, sensitivity to draft can lead to thermal dissatisfaction [39], as evident in a minority of current-study participants. More simply stated, some participants who were not hot to begin with may have felt overcooled by the fan. Supporting this finding, other researchers have noted that thermal discomfort due to a fan’s cooling effect can be attributed to feeling thermally neutral before fan interaction [40,41].

Fan visibility, specifically high versus low, was manipulated to understand its effect on temperature-adjustment behaviors. In the two high-visibility conditions, the fan cue was salient, i.e., the stimulus was salient to the perceiver [4245]. Temperature adjustments were marginally affected by fan visibility. That is, participants set slightly higher temperatures in the two high-visibility conditions (23.5 °C/74.3 °F) than in the two low-visibility conditions (22.8 °C/73.0 °F). However, this marginal relationship was not preserved when examining mean temperature shifts. That is, there was no significant difference between participant-set temperatures in the high-visibility conditions minus baseline temperatures, and participant-set temperatures in the low-visibility conditions minus baseline temperatures. This marginal visibility effect is consistent with recent behavioral priming meta-analyses where most effects are small [46]. Thus, priming participants with high-fan visibility alone may not be sufficient to cue energy-efficient temperature adjustments in warm ambient temperatures.

4.2 Change in Temperature Selections and Design Implications.

Temperature selection was affected by the intervention interfaces, which combined fan feel and visibility parameters. Analyzing mean raw temperatures revealed a significant relationship between temperature adjustments and intervention interface. Participants’ mean raw temperatures were highest in the feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) condition (24.4 °C/76.0 °F), versus baseline (22.0 °C/71.7 °F), feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV) condition (23.2 °C/73.7 °F), don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV) condition (22.7 °C/72.8 °F), and don't-feel-fan low-visibility (DFF + LV) condition (22.4 °C/72.4 °F). These results support that increased air speed during thermostat interaction encourages users toward energy-efficient temperature adjustments.

A theme that emerged in the open-ended responses was visibility-induced frustration for the don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV) condition. If high fan visibility alone primes people to feel cool, this condition would prompt higher temperature selection. Our results showed a marginal effect of visibility. Again, average temperature selections in the don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV) condition (22.7 °C/72.8 °F) were lower (worse) than in the feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) condition (24.4 °C/76.0 °F) and feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV) condition (23.2 °C/73.7 °F). Nine participants reported that high-fan visibility caused frustration (or a similar reaction). Seven of the nine participants also commented that visibility-induced frustration led to lower temperature selection. This effect is articulated by a participant as, “The promise of a breeze locked behind glass frustrated me, prompting me to select a lower temperature in hopes of getting relief from the heat.” Furthermore, one participant explained how the don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV) interface affected their behavior as, “When I can see the fan clearly but cannot feel the air from the fan, I feel kind of frustrated (even more frustrated than when the fans are behind white bars when I cannot see it clearly) so I decreased the temperature …” Thus, despite quantitative analysis showing little effect of fan visibility alone on temperature selection, the lack of fan feel in the don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV) condition frustrated some users. Therefore, fan visibility without fan feel should be avoided in actual interventions and designs.

4.3 Incorrect Conceptual Model of Heat Delivery.

This study also administered five questions to gauge knowledge regarding energy myths based on Pritoni et al.’s assessment of household behavior [36]. Current-study participants were divided on their understanding of heat delivery. About 52% of participants erroneously, and consistently, responded “True” to three questions explaining heat delivery through the gas-pedal metaphor. That is, participants believed that selecting significantly higher or lower temperatures would heat or cool them quicker, respectively. Overall, our study supports Pritoni et al.’s evaluation that household occupants hold misconceptions about thermostat control and energy use [36]. Thus, an incorrect conceptual model of heat delivery may be another barrier to energy-efficient temperature setting.

4.4 Study Limitations.

In hindsight, a study limitation is that fans were on the same settings in the feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) and feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV) conditions; however, the grill cover in the low-visibility (LV) condition partially blocked air flow, reducing the fan’s effect. This difference in felt air speed between conditions (see Table 2) may have affected study results. Specifically, participants’ mean raw temperature setting was 1.25 °C/2.25 °F higher in the feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV) condition than in the feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV) condition. Because of the unintended difference in felt air speed, it is difficult to conclude whether it was this difference or the difference in fan visibility that led to the change in temperature selections. Future iterations should address this difference in felt air speed to more precisely study fan feel versus fan visibility on temperature selection. Furthermore, given a few participants’ thermal discomfort (i.e., they reported feeling cold) in the feel-fan (FF) conditions, reduced fan speed may achieve the same effect with less thermal discomfort.

Table 2

Felt air speed by condition and fan setting

Feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV)Feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV)Don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV)Don't-feel-fan low-visibility (DFF + LV)
Fan setting3 of 33 of 31 of 30 of 3
Felt air speed∼1.7 to 1.8 m/s∼0.9 to 1.4 m/s0 m/s0 m/s
Feel-fan high-visibility (FF + HV)Feel-fan low-visibility (FF + LV)Don't-feel-fan high-visibility (DFF + HV)Don't-feel-fan low-visibility (DFF + LV)
Fan setting3 of 33 of 31 of 30 of 3
Felt air speed∼1.7 to 1.8 m/s∼0.9 to 1.4 m/s0 m/s0 m/s

Another study limitation is that participants’ behavior was measured over a period of minutes. To better understand the intervention’s effect, thermal selections over longer time scales (for example, throughout the day) and more varied conditions remain to be tested. In addition, the intervention was intended to provide brief thermal relief during user-thermostat interaction. Not tested, however, was how the fan’s absence post-interaction affects thermal comfort and acceptance.

The small and relatively homogeneous sample of university students makes it difficult to generalize the findings. The oldest two of 23 participants indicated they were in the 25–34-years-old age range. Future iterations should test the intervention’s efficacy on a more heterogeneous set of participants.

Finally, indoor temperatures during the study ranged from 24–27 °C (75.2–80.6 °F). While these ranges represent normal temperature fluctuations during the summer months at the study location, different indoor temperatures may reduce the ability to compare results between participants. Future studies should maintain control of the indoor temperature while participants are undergoing the experiment.

Implementing an energy-conserving intervention must consume less energy than the energy that the intervention is intended to conserve. Electromechanical fans alone deliver instant, targeted thermal relief, and generally consume much less energy than using A/C to cool an entire space [6]. Additionally, fans may be utilized to widen indoor temperature bands, further contributing to energy savings [6].

Of note, while current-study fans were placed directly above the thermostat to observe the effects of both fan feel and fan visibility, other configurations may also be effective. Fans integrated with the thermostat may consume less energy, as these fans can be turned on when user-thermostat interaction begins. Since fan visibility did not prove to be important, other fan locations are possible. For example, a standalone fan placed anywhere, provided it could be felt while setting the thermostat, may achieve comparable results without requiring a reconfigured thermostat.

5 Conclusion

Increasing global energy demand for A/C emphasizes the urgency associated with developing behavior-change interventions to reduce its consumption. The current study offers a potential strategy to address energy-inefficient temperature selection. Reducing energy use in heating, ventilation and air-conditioning via energy-conscious user behavior can lead to significant savings in resource consumption [47,48].

The current work examined how temperature decisions can be modified by altering the decision environment. Rather than trying to change people’s beliefs and values around energy conservation, modifying the environment (in which energy-consumption decisions are made) may be more effective. Specifically, this study demonstrated the effectiveness of using a fan during thermostat interaction to encourage energy-efficient temperature selection in warm ambient conditions. Of note, fan feel is more important than fan visibility. In fact, fan visibility without fan feel should be avoided.

Participants made temperature adjustments on actual thermostats, rather than responding to researcher questions, thus reducing the often-reported disconnect between intention and action [49]. Furthermore, awareness of current ambient room temperature mimics real-world conditions present during thermostat interaction. Combined, these study parameters led to a more realistic experimental scenario that is more likely to generalize outside the laboratory setting.

This work offers a behavior-change intervention to encourage energy consciousness toward a more sustainable future. While the study results are promising, more work is required to understand the intervention’s longer-term effects. Future work should also test the intervention in more naturalistic settings.

One of the United Nations’ sustainable development goals is to take urgent action to address climate change and its effects [50]. Climate-change-driven planet warming threatens human health as critical thermal limits of the body could be surpassed [51]. Ironically, overuse of A/C hastens overall planet warming, a problem the current work sought to address.

Acknowledgment

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC), the Collaborative Specialization in Psychology, Psychiatry and Engineering (PsychEng), and the XSeed funding program at the University of Toronto.

Conflict of Interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

Ethics Approval

This research was approved by the University of Toronto Institutional Review Board (Protocol No. 35708).

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and supporting the findings of this article are obtainable from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

1.
Salamanca
,
F.
,
Georgescu
,
M.
,
Mahalov
,
A.
,
Moustaoui
,
M.
, and
Wang
,
M.
,
2014
, “
Anthropogenic Heating of the Urban Environment Due to Air Conditioning
,”
J. Geophys. Res.: Atmos.
,
119
(
10
), pp.
5949
5965
.
2.
Obringer
,
R.
,
Nateghi
,
R.
,
Maia-Silva
,
D.
,
Mukherjee
,
S.
,
Vineeth
,
C. R.
,
McRoberts
,
D. B.
, and
Kumar
,
R.
,
2022
, “
Implications of Increasing Household Air Conditioning Use Across the United States Under a Warming Climate
,”
Earth’s Future
,
10
(
1
). .
e2021EF002434
.
3.
Lawson
,
A.
,
2023
, “UK Heatwave Prompts National Grid to Fire Up Coal Plant to Meet Aircon Demand,” The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/environment/2023/jun/12/uk-heatwave-prompts-order-to-fire-up-coal-plant-to-meet-aircon-demand
4.
Dong
,
Y.
,
Coleman
,
M.
, and
Miller
,
S. A.
,
2021
, “
Greenhouse Gas Emissions From Air Conditioning and Refrigeration Service Expansion in Developing Countries
,”
Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour.
,
46
(
1
), pp.
59
83
.
5.
Lundgren-Kownacki
,
K.
,
Hornyanszky
,
E. D.
,
Chu
,
T. A.
,
Olsson
,
J. A.
, and
Becker
,
P.
,
2017
, “
Challenges of Using Air Conditioning in an Increasingly Hot Climate
,”
Int. J. Biometeorol.
,
62
(
3
), pp.
401
412
.
6.
Jay
,
O.
,
Hoelzl
,
R.
,
Weets
,
J.
,
Morris
,
N.
,
English
,
T.
,
Nybo
,
L.
,
Niu
,
J.
,
de Dear
,
R.
, and
Capon
,
A.
,
2019
, “
Fanning as an Alternative to Air Conditioning—A Sustainable Solution for Reducing Indoor Occupational Heat Stress
,”
Energy Build.
,
193
(
4
), pp.
92
98
.
7.
Srivastava
,
J.
, and
Shu
,
L. H.
,
2013
, “
Encouraging Resource-Conscious Behavior Through Product Design: The Principle of Discretization
,”
ASME J. Mech. Des.
,
135
(
6
), p.
061002
.
8.
Srivastava
,
J.
, and
Shu
,
L. H.
,
2013
, “
Affordances and Product Design to Support Environmentally Conscious Behavior
,”
ASME J. Mech. Des.
,
135
(
10
), p.
101006
.
9.
Telenko
,
C.
,
O’Rourke
,
J. M.
,
Seepersad
,
C. C.
, and
Webber
,
M. E.
,
2016
, “
A Compilation of Design for Environment Guidelines
,”
ASME J. Mech. Des.
,
138
(
3
), p.
031102
.
10.
Withanage
,
C.
,
Hölttä-Otto
,
K.
,
Otto
,
K.
, and
Wood
,
K.
,
2016
, “
Design for Sustainable Use of Appliances: A Framework Based on User Behavior Observations
,”
ASME J. Mech. Des.
,
138
(
10
), p.
101102
. .
11.
She
,
J.
, and
Macdonald
,
E.
,
2018
, “
Exploring the Effects of a Product’s Sustainability Triggers on Pro-environmental Decision-Making
,”
ASME J. Mech. Des.
,
140
(
1
), p.
011102
.
12.
Pakravan
,
M. H.
, and
MacCarty
,
N. A.
,
2020
, “
Design for Clean Technology Adoption: Integration of Usage Context, User Behavior, and Technology Performance in Design
,”
ASME J. Mech. Des.
,
142
(
9
), p.
091402
.
13.
Peffer
,
T.
,
Pritoni
,
M.
,
Meier
,
A.
,
Aragon
,
C.
, and
Perry
,
D.
,
2011
, “
How People Use Thermostats in Homes: A Review
,”
Build. Environ.
,
46
(
12
), pp.
2529
2541
.
14.
Yang
,
R.
, and
Newman
,
M. W.
,
2013
, “
Learning From a Learning Thermostat: Lessons for Intelligent Systems for the Home
,”
Proceedings of the 2013 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing
,
Zurich, Switzerland
,
Sept. 8–12
, pp.
93
102
.
15.
Stopps
,
H.
, and
Touchie
,
M. F.
,
2021
, “
Residential Smart Thermostat Use: An Exploration of Thermostat Programming, Environmental Attitudes, and the Influence of Smart Controls on Energy Savings
,”
Energy Build.
,
238
(
6
), p.
110834
.
16.
Pisharoty
,
D.
,
Yang
,
R.
,
Newman
,
M. W.
, and
Whitehouse
,
K.
,
2015
, “
ThermoCoach: Reducing Home Energy Consumption With Personalized Thermostat Recommendations
,”
Proceedings of the 2nd ACM International Conference on Embedded Systems for Energy-Efficient Built Environments
,
Seoul, South Korea
,
Nov. 4–5
, pp.
201
210
.
17.
Steg
,
L.
, and
Vlek
,
C.
,
2009
, “
Encouraging Pro-environmental Behaviour: An Integrative Review and Research Agenda
,”
J. Environ. Psychol.
,
29
(
3
), pp.
309
317
.
18.
Shu
,
L. H.
,
Duflou
,
J.
,
Herrmann
,
C.
,
Sakao
,
T.
,
Shimomura
,
Y.
,
De Bock
,
Y.
, and
Srivastava
,
J.
,
2017
, “
Design for Reduced Resource Consumption During the Use Phase of Products
,”
CIRP Ann.
,
66
(
2
), pp.
635
658
.
19.
Abrahamse
,
W.
, and
Matthies
,
E.
,
2018
, “Informational Strategies to Promote Pro-environmental Behaviour,”
Environmental Psychology: An Introduction
, L. Steg and J .I. M. de Groot, eds.,
John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
,
Hoboken, NJ
, pp.
261
272
.
20.
Zachrisson
,
J.
, and
Boks
,
C.
,
2010
, “
When to Apply Different Design for Sustainable Behavior Strategies?
Proceedings of the ERSCP-EMSU Conference
,
Delft, The Netherlands
,
Oct. 25–29
.
21.
Bhamra
,
T.
,
Lilley
,
D.
, and
Tang
,
T.
,
2011
, “
Design for Sustainable Behaviour: Using Products to Change Consumer Behaviour
,”
Des. J.
,
14
(
4
), pp.
427
445
.
22.
Yang
,
R.
,
Pisharoty
,
D.
,
Montazeri
,
S.
,
Whitehouse
,
K.
, and
Newman
,
M. W.
,
2016
, “
How Does Eco-Coaching Help to Save Energy? Assessing a Recommendation System for Energy-Efficient Thermostat Scheduling
,”
Proceedings of the 2016 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing
,
Heidelberg, Germany
,
Sept. 12–16
, pp.
1176
1187
.
23.
De Bock
,
Y.
,
Auquilla
,
A.
,
Bracquené
,
E.
,
Nowé
,
A.
, and
Duflou
,
J. R.
,
2021
, “
The Energy Saving Potential of Retrofitting a Smart Heating System: A Residence Hall Pilot Study
,”
Sustain. Comput.: Inform. Syst.
,
31
(
2
), p.
100585
.
24.
Mertens
,
S.
,
Herberz
,
M.
,
Hahnel
,
U. J.
, and
Brosch
,
T.
,
2021
, “
The Effectiveness of Nudging: A Meta-Analysis of Choice Architecture Interventions Across Behavioral Domains
,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
,
119
(
1
), p.
e2107346118
.
25.
Evans
,
J. S.
,
2008
, “
Dual-Processing Accounts of Reasoning, Judgment, and Social Cognition
,”
Annu. Rev. Psychol.
,
59
(
1
), pp.
255
278
.
26.
Simon
,
H. A.
,
1955
, “
A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice
,”
Q. J. Econ.
,
69
(
1
), pp.
99
118
.
27.
Simon
,
H. A.
,
1982
,
Models of Bounded Rationality
,
MIT Press
,
Cambridge, MA
.
28.
Liu
,
J. H.
, and
Sibley
,
C. G.
,
2004
, “
Attitudes and Behavior in Social Space: Public Good Interventions Based on Shared Representations and Environmental Influences
,”
J. Environ. Psychol.
,
24
(
3
), pp.
373
384
.
29.
Halabieh, S., and Shu, L. H., 2024, “Reducing Waste Outflow to Motivate Water Conservation,”
ASME J. Mech. Des.
, 146(2).
30.
Fishman
,
J.
,
Lushin
,
V.
, and
Mandell
,
D. S.
,
2020
, “
Predicting Implementation: Comparing Validated Measures of Intention and Assessing the Role of Motivation When Designing Behavioral Interventions
,”
Implement. Sci. Commun.
,
1
(
1
), p.
81
.
31.
Paniccia
,
C.
, and
Shu
,
L. H.
,
2018
, “
Message Framing to Reduce Automobile Idling
,”
Proceedings of the ASME 2018 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference
,
Quebec City, Quebec, Canada
,
Aug. 26–29
,
Paper No. DETC2018-85679
.
32.
Canova
,
L.
,
Bobbio
,
A.
, and
Manganelli
,
A. M.
,
2023
, “
Sustainable Purchase Intentions: The Role of Moral Norm and Social Dominance Orientation in the Theory of Planned Behavior Applied to the Case of Fair Trade Products
,”
Sustain. Dev.
,
31
(
2
), pp.
1069
1083
.
33.
He
,
Y.
,
Li
,
N.
,
Li
,
N.
,
Li
,
J.
,
Yan
,
J.
, and
Tan
,
C.
,
2018
, “
Control Behaviors and Thermal Comfort in a Shared Room With Desk Fans and Adjustable Thermostat
,”
Build. Environ.
,
136
, pp.
213
226
.
34.
He
,
Y.
,
Li
,
N.
,
Zhang
,
H.
,
Han
,
Y.
,
Lu
,
J.
, and
Zhou
,
L.
,
2020
, “
Air-Conditioning Use Behaviors When Elevated Air Movement Is Available
,”
Energy Build.
,
225
, p.
110370
.
35.
Steg
,
L.
,
Perlaviciute
,
G.
,
van der Werff
,
E.
, and
Lurvink
,
J.
,
2014
, “
The Significance of Hedonic Values for Environmentally Relevant Attitudes, Preferences, and Actions
,”
Environ. Behav.
,
46
(
2
), pp.
163
192
.
36.
Pritoni
,
M.
,
Meier
,
A. K.
,
Aragon
,
C.
,
Perry
,
D.
, and
Peffer
,
T.
,
2015
, “
Energy Efficiency and the Misuse of Programmable Thermostats: The Effectiveness of Crowdsourcing for Understanding Household Behavior
,”
Energy Res. Soc. Sci.
,
8
, pp.
190
197
.
37.
ANSI/ASHRAE
,
2017
, “
American National Standards Institute (ANSI)/American Society of Heating, Refrigerating and Air-Conditioning Engineers (ASHRAE) Standard 55: Thermal Environmental Conditions for Human Occupancy
.”
38.
Zhai
,
Y.
,
Zhang
,
H.
,
Zhang
,
Y.
,
Pasut
,
W.
,
Arens
,
E.
, and
Meng
,
Q.
,
2013
, “
Comfort Under Personally Controlled Air Movement in Warm and Humid Environments
,”
Build. Environ.
,
65
, pp.
109
117
.
39.
Fanger
,
P. O.
, and
Christensen
,
N. K.
,
1986
, “
Perception of Draught in Ventilated Spaces
,”
Ergonomics
,
29
(
2
), pp.
215
235
.
40.
Melikov
,
A. K.
, and
Nielsen
,
J. B.
,
1989
, “
Local Thermal Discomfort Due to Draft and Vertical Temperature Difference in Rooms With Displacement Ventilation
,”
ASHRAE Trans.
,
95
(
2
), pp.
1050
1057
.
41.
Parkinson
,
T.
, and
de Dear
,
R.
,
2015
, “
Thermal Pleasure in Built Environments: Physiology of Alliesthesia
,”
Build. Res. Inf.
,
43
(
3
), pp.
288
301
.
42.
Dehaene
,
S.
,
Changeux
,
J.-P.
,
Naccache
,
L.
,
Sackur
,
J.
, and
Sergent
,
C.
,
2006
, “
Conscious, Preconscious, and Subliminal Processing: A Testable Taxonomy
,”
Trends Cognit. Sci.
,
10
(
5
), pp.
204
211
.
43.
Dehaene
,
S.
, and
Naccache
,
L.
,
2001
, “
Towards a Cognitive Neuroscience of Consciousness: Basic Evidence and a Workspace Framework
,”
Cognition
,
79
(
1–2
), pp.
1
37
.
44.
Dehaene
,
S.
,
Sergent
,
C.
, and
Changeux
,
J.-P.
,
2003
, “
A Neuronal Network Model Linking Subjective Reports and Objective Physiological Data During Conscious Perception
,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
,
100
(
14
), pp.
8520
8525
.
45.
Doyen
,
S.
,
Klein
,
O.
,
Pichon
,
C.-L.
, and
Cleeremans
,
A.
,
2012
, “
Behavioral Priming: It's All in the Mind, But Whose Mind?
,”
PLoS One
,
7
(
1
), p.
e29081
.
46.
Weingarten
,
E.
,
Chen
,
Q.
,
McAdams
,
M.
,
Yi
,
J.
,
Hepler
,
J.
, and
Albarracín
,
D.
,
2016
, “
From Primed Concepts to Action: A Meta-Analysis of the Behavioral Effects of Incidentally Presented Words
,”
Psychol. Bull.
,
142
(
5
), pp.
472
497
.
47.
Wu
,
W.
,
Skye
,
H. M.
, and
Domanski
,
P. A.
,
2018
, “
Selecting HVAC Systems to Achieve Comfortable and Cost-Effective Residential Net-Zero Energy Buildings
,”
Appl. Energy
,
212
, pp.
577
591
.
48.
Stopps
,
H.
, and
Touchie
,
M. F.
,
2019
, “
Reduction of HVAC System Runtime Due to Occupancy-Controlled Smart Thermostats in Contemporary Multi-Unit Residential Building Suites
,”
IOP Conf. Ser.: Mater. Sci. Eng.
,
609
(
6
), p.
062013
.
49.
Sheeran
,
P.
, and
Webb
,
T. L.
,
2016
, “
The Intention-Behavior Gap
,”
Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass
,
10
(
9
), pp.
503
518
.
50.
United Nations
,
2015
, “Goal 13: Take Urgent Action to Combat Climate Change and Its Impacts,” https://www.un.org/sustainabledevelopment/climate-change/
51.
Powis
,
C. M.
,
Byrne
,
D.
,
Zobel
,
Z.
,
Gassert
,
K. N.
,
Lute
,
A. C.
, and
Schwalm
,
C. R.
,
2023
, “
Observational and Model Evidence Together Support Wide-Spread Exposure to Noncompensable Heat Under Continued Global Warming
,”
Sci. Adv.
,
9
(
36
).